home

Chimps, Chickens, and Communication

A new study published in Current Biology proves (as if more evidence were needed) that, in the words of zoologist Matthew Cobb, “humans aren’t quite so unique, after all.” The so-called news is that chimps will warn other chimps about a hazard if and only if the other chimps don’t already know about the danger. This adds to the evidence that chimps not only use sound-symbols to convey meaning, as we do, but also that they have what psychologists call theory of mind: an awareness that others have mental states (such as knowing something or not) different than one’s own.

Chickens are similarly flexible in their production of and responses to alarm cries. When a rooster spots a hawk or other potential predator in the sky, he shouts a sound-symbol that actually sounds a lot like the word “HAWK.” Everybody runs for cover without delay. Other roosters shout too, whether or not they’ve seen the threatening shadow in the sky. Hens join in, clucking in low foreboding tones. Nobody shuts up or steps back out into the open until the rooster who raised the original warning crows an “all clear” signal.

In contrast, if a rooster spots a fox or other potential predator on the ground, he makes a different warning sound. Then, everybody stops moving and looks around. The calls of roosters, then, are what researchers call “functionally referential, encoding sufficient information about the circumstances of production for conspecifics to respond appropriately, even in the absence of contextual cues potentially provided by the non-vocal behavior of the sender.”

None of this is surprising. The ancestors of modern chickens evolved in the jungles of South Asia, where flocks did (and still do) forage amid dense foliage that prevents them from maintaining visual contact. Such circumstances presumably led to the development of language among our own ancestors. It’s only human hubris that leads us to be surprised when other animals turn out to have utilized some of the same strategies for survival as we did.

And so we demand proof. And so scientists set up studies.

The good news is that, in the case of the chimps, the researchers did not significantly interfere with the animals or their habitat in order to collect their data. They merely placed a replica of a poisonous viper along a path frequented by chimps and watched what happened. Since watching out for, avoiding, and warning others about such environmental hazards is part of the everyday lives of those chimps, that was a relatively unobtrusive way to learn something about them. It’s still scaring somebody to study their reaction but, given that such scares are an everyday occurrence in the lives of free-living animals, probably not injurious.

That’s not the case with the study of rooster alarm cries, which was done by researchers who have repeatedly used laboratory experiments to demonstrate that chickens have communications capacities far more sophisticated than most people presume. Raising, yet again, the question of what these scientists might be thinking about their own right to experiment on these sentient beings.

Ironically, research concerning the capabilities of chimpanzees helped to supply some of the data leading the U.S. to suspend funding for any new biomedical research using chimps as subjects. Anybody who has read about any of the horrific experiments to which chimpanzees have been subjected has to cheer that decision. At the same time, that and other bans or limitations on the use of great apes as research subjects often depend on the argument that, because they are closest to us, great apes are so superior to other animals as to be exempt.

Chickens are also exempt, but in a different way. Rodents and birds are specifically exempted from the animal welfare regulations that minimally mitigate the suffering that can be inflicted upon animals used in research. (See our page on birds and vivisection for more information.)

Some activists, such as Ingrid Newkirk of PETA, argue that any regulation that in any way establishes any sort of right for any animal represents a crack in the foundation of the justification of human exploitation of animals. Over time, the theory goes, those cracks will bring the building down. Others argue that protecting some animals, but not others, leaves the structure unchallenged, especially when the justification for special protection is that the protected animal is especially “like us.” What do you think?

4 comments to Chimps, Chickens, and Communication

  • CQ
    The latter.

    My reasoning rests on the rights principle that David Cantor, founder of Responsible Policies for Animals (www.RPAforAll.com), espouses.

    Cantor wrote in his most recent newsletter: “To advance our goal of establishing justice, liberty, and wellbeing for all -– establishing basic autonomy, ecology, and dignity rights of all animals –- we must dedicate ourselves, in 2012, to strategies, tactics, and language that establish rights….”

    He cites the need to “debunk false beliefs: that only human beings are persons; that humans are sapient (knowing and wise) and other animals are not; that nonhuman animals do not think or feel; that other animals lack morality; that humans evolved as hunter-gatherers and omnivores and it is natural, normal, and good for them to use other animals. Failure to accord rights that others need comes from misunderstanding of self and others.”

    And he asks readers to “help people see that extending basic rights beyond the one animal species currently possessing them is the only solution to the big human problems –- poor health, poverty, war, overpopulation, breakup of families, food and water shortages, destruction of the natural world, and injustice against nonhuman animals.”

    I especially like his strategy of “rejecting regulating injustice in favor of establishing rights to eliminate injustice.”

    RPA’s Draft Bill of Animal Rights can be found here: http://www.rpaforall.org/billofanimalrights.html

    Clearly, Cantor has come far in his thinking from his days as a PETA employee!

  • Nancy
    Since peta kills sentient animals,and is a welfarist group with stocks in slaughterhouses, anything newkirk and her ilk say is nullified.
  • bravebird
    pattrice here: Richard Nixon resigned in 1974. That’s true no matter who says it. So, I try to assess arguments on their merits (or lack thereof) rather than the characteristics of the person making them.

    What data do we have that might help us decide which strategies are most likely to lead to the liberation of all animals in laboratories?

    Me personally, I’m skeptical of a sole focus on “rights.” Right now, for the first time in history, enslavement of human beings is illegal in every country in the world. At the same time, there are now more human beings in slavery than at any other time in human history. Similarly, the codification of a “right to food” in the constitutions of several countries has not, in fact, led to the end of hunger in those countries.

    Here’s an article I wrote about the question of “rights” as a possibly problematic strategy for animal liberation: What’s Wrong with Rights

    Also, in our archive there are a couple of reports intended to help animal advocates think through strategic questions similar to those raised by the prospect of special protections for chimps.

  • Nancy
    That may be so but I am sure Hitler stated some truths in his days but no one quotes him to bolster an argument. Focusing on Newkirk as an “activist” gives her and her organization credibility. I am sure you are well aware that peta routinely kills nonhuman animals and owns stocks in slaughterhouses. Their news of starting a porn channel interspersed with messages of animal exploitation is a horrible idea in so many ways.

Leave a Reply

You can use these HTML tags

<a href="" title=""> <abbr title=""> <acronym title=""> <b> <blockquote cite=""> <cite> <code> <del datetime=""> <em> <i> <q cite=""> <s> <strike> <strong>